A well-written __________ should strike a balance between using logical and emotional reasoning.

  • Journal List
  • Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci
  • v.7[4]; 2012 Apr
  • PMC3324564

Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012 Apr; 7[4]: 380–392.

Abstract

The brain’s default network [DN] is comprised of several cortical regions demonstrating robust intrinsic connectivity at rest. The authors sought to examine the differential effects of emotional reasoning and reasoning under certainty upon the DN through the employment of an event-related fMRI design in healthy participants. Participants were presented with syllogistic arguments which were organized into a 2 × 2 factorial design in which the first factor was emotional salience and the second factor was certainty/uncertainty. We demonstrate that regions of the DN were activated both during reasoning that is emotionally salient and during reasoning which is more certain, suggesting that these processes are neurally instantiated on a network level. In addition, we present evidence that emotional reasoning preferentially activates the dorsomedial [dMPFC] subsystem of the DN, whereas reasoning in the context of certainty activates areas specific to the DN’s medial temporal [MTL] subsystem. We postulate that emotional reasoning mobilizes the dMPFC subsystem of the DN because this type of reasoning relies upon the recruitment of introspective and self-relevant data such as personal bias and temperament. In contrast, activation of the MTL subsystem during certainty argues that this form of reasoning involves the recruitment of mnemonic and semantic associations to derive conclusions.

Keywords: default network, emotional reasoning, certainty, uncertainty, fMRI

INTRODUCTION

Reasoning is undoubtedly a heteromodal process that involves the recruitment of several cognitive domains. These include, but are not limited to: working memory, episodic retrieval, analogy and abstraction. Still, some authors have pointed to supramodal neural networks responsible for reasoning [Rodriguez-Moreno and Hirsch, 2009], and there is evidence to suggest that the type of network being recruited is dependent on the ‘content’ of the logical problem to be solved [Goel, 2007a]. Two types of content-the emotionality of the logical argument, and the extent to which the reasoner is certain or uncertain about the conclusion-seem to have particularly strong influences upon the reasoning process. This is possibly due to the fact that in both of these types of reasoning, the reasoner must draw upon some form of personal or introspective data in order to derive a conclusion. With respect to the effects of emotion upon reasoning, there is mounting evidence to suggest that reasoning can be biased by an emotional investment in the conclusion being drawn. For instance, behavioral studies [Blanchette and Richards, 2004; Blanchette, 2006; Oaksford and Chater, 2001] have found that subjects were more likely to make invalid deductions while reasoning about emotional material in comparison to neutral materials. Still, emotional reasoning should not be conceptualized as simply being reasoning in presence of emotional [or semantically rich] materials. Instead, we define ‘emotional reasoning’ as being a form of logical reasoning in which a conclusion is drawn that is implicitly influenced by the affective state induced in the reasoner. In a related manner, reasoning also appears to heavily depend upon the reasoner’s prior knowledge of, and thus certainty of, the material contained within a logical argument. For example, one study involving patients with prefrontal lesions found a hemispheric specialization for reasoning in certain and uncertain contexts [Goel et al., 2007b]. Although it is tempting to define ‘certainty reasoning’ as reasoning which is less cognitively taxing, we here define it as being a form of logical reasoning in which the conclusion is influenced by mnemonic and semantic associations employed by the reasoner.

Still, the neural instantiation of emotional and certainty reasoning, as here defined, remain poorly understood. One of the few studies to examine emotional reasoning was performed by Goel and Dolan [2003]. These authors demonstrated reciprocal activations between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [DLPFC] and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vMPFC], based upon whether subjects reasoned about neutral material or emotional material, respectively. The authors went on to argue that the association of vMPFC with emotional reasoning resonated with several other studies of emotional decision-making. For example, vMPFC is critical to reward-based learning [Hansel and Van Kanel, 2008; Knutson et al., 2005; McClure et al., 2004]. In addition, Damasio and colleagues’ showed that the orbitofrontal cortex coordinates somatic markers which convey emotional and motivational relevance during decision-making [Bechara et al., 1997; Damasio, 1996]. Such a role for the vMPFC in emotional decision-making is also supported by lesion data, and on the basis of rich anatomical interconnections between vMPFC and areas which accumulate emotionally salient information from visceral structures and the outside world [Fellows, 2007].

However, Goel and Dolan used a ‘deductive’ and not an ‘inductive’ reasoning task. This distinction is critical, as inductive reasoning more closely parallels real-world judgments, where the reasoner can only assess the probability of a given conclusion, and not its inherent validity [Goel and Dolan, 2000]. That is, deduction relies upon the application of premises contained within a logical argument, and thus is often predicated on syntactic and linguistic features of said argument [Goel, 2007]. Induction, on the other hand, involves the extrapolation of given premises in order to formulate new rules and hypotheses. In this way, during induction the reasoner is more prone to draw upon his/her own ‘personal’ knowledge and experience, than in deduction, where one is more likely to depend upon ‘external’ and universally established rules [Goel et al., 1997]. Therefore, induction is far more likely to be biased by content such as emotional valence and level of certainty.

We thus set out to employ an inductive task to examine the neural instantiation of the effects of both emotion and certainty upon reasoning. We hypothesized that each of these factors would result in activation of a network of areas, [rather than one circumscribed region such as vMPFC] and that these areas would anatomically overlap with those of the brain’s Default Network [DN]. We based this hypothesis on two general properties about the DN: [i] that the use of an inductive task would be more likely to activate the DN than a deductive one and [ii] that emotional content and certainty during induction would both cause preferential recruitment of the network. First, prior evidence suggests that the DN might be involved in the inductive process. The network is thought to mediate the computation of probabilities [Raichle and Gusnard, 2005], a process central to inductive reasoning [Goel and Dolan, 2004]. Also, the DN relies upon mnemonic associations to compute such probabilities [Bar, 2009; Bar et al., 2007], and Corcoran [2010] has argued that this process parallels the hypothesis generation characteristic of inductive reasoning. Also, when directly contrasting deduction and induction, inductive reasoning activated key anterior components of the DN, specifically Brodmann areas 9, 24 and 32 [Goel et al., 1997]. Second, extant data supports a potential role for the DN in emotional reasoning and for reasoning with certainty. The DN has repeatedly been shown to activate during decision-making which relies upon implicit social knowledge and self-reflection [Schneider et al., 2008; Gusnard et al., 2001], thus making the case for it to be so activated during emotional reasoning. There is also evidence to suggest that reasoning which relies on well established semantic associations and prior knowledge [and thus greater certainty] activates components of the network-specifically the medial temporal lobe [Bar, 2009].

We went on to formulate more specific hypothesis with respect to the ways in which the DN would be activated by emotional and certainty reasoning. That is, we theorized that two different network subsystems would be engaged during each of these conditions. Andrews-Hanna and colleagues [2010] recently differentiated two subsystems of the DN: a dorsomedial [dMPFC] subsystem [comprised of the dMPFC, the temporoparietal junction [TPJ], the lateral temporal cortex [LTC] and the temporal pole [TP]], which is activated when affective information is referenced to the self, and during the reflection of one’s mental state [and the mental state of others]; and a medial temporal [MTL] subsystem. The latter subsystem, comprised of the hippocampal formation [HF], vMPFC, the retrosplenial cortex [RsP], the parahippocampus [PHC] and the posterior lateral inferior parietal cortex [pIPL], appears to be involved in using mnemonic information to simulate future contexts. This raises the possibility that in the context of high levels of mnemonic association to, or semantic knowledge about, an inductive argument, the MTL subsystem might be mobilized. The authors therefore hypothesized that emotional reasoning, relative to neutral reasoning, would increase activity in the dorsomedial subsystem of the DN; whereas reasoning in the context of high levels of certainty, relative to reasoning in the context of uncertainty, would increase activity in the medial temporal subsystem of the DN. To test these hypotheses, we employed a 2 × 2 factorial design in which inductive arguments varied based on emotional salience and on the basis of certainty level. The former was accomplished by dividing arguments into emotional and neutral categories. The latter was accomplished by partitioning arguments into ‘certain’ and ‘uncertain’ categories.

METHODS

Subject participants

Twenty right-handed [confirmed by the Edinburgh handedness inventory] healthy adults [10 females; ages 21–32 mean = 24.25, s.d = 3.04] participated in the experiment. All subjects were native English speakers. Participants were recruited through the Division of Psychiatric Neuroscience Research and Neurotherapeutics at the Massachusetts General Hospital via bulletin board notices within the hospital. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in accordance with the guidelines of the Subcommittee on Human Studies of the Massachusetts General Hospital. Participants were screened to ensure the absence of any neurological or psychiatric condition via the use of clinical interviews performed by a physician as well as via the use of the following diagnostic assessment batteries: the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV [SCID] [First et al., 1995], the Beck depression Inventory [Beck et al., 1996] and the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale [Hamilton, 1960].

Task

We patterned the experimental task from that used by Goel and Dolan [2003]. Subjects were presented with logical arguments in the form of a syllogism, which is a three sentence logical argument in which the first two sentences constitute premises of the argument and the third sentence represents the conclusion [Goel, 2007a]. As an example, subjects were presented with the following two premises: ‘Molly plays a rare sport’ and ‘Molly is rather tall’ and this was followed by the conclusion: ‘Most women who play this sport are tall’. Upon presentation of the third sentence subjects were asked to make a button press based on whether the third sentence was thought to be ‘probably true’ or ‘probably false’, given that the first two sentences [i.e. premises] were true. A total of 256 syllogistic arguments were presented in a 2 × 2 factorial design [Tables 1 and 2]. The first factor was emotional salience. That is, half of all arguments [128] contained emotionally valenced material whereas the other half contained material that was expected to be emotionally neutral. Of note, all of the emotional arguments involved negative, and not positive, emotions [e.g. horror, grief, disgust]. The second factor was level of certainty, such that half of all arguments [128] were created in a way that the correct answer was ambiguous and the other half were devised in such a manner that the correct answer would be obvious and rapidly forthcoming. The former was accomplished by providing less clueing in the first two premises and/or by discussing themes with which subjects were not expected to have a priori knowledge. Certainty, in contrast, was achieved by manufacturing arguments in which subjects could draw a rapid conclusion based on common semantic knowledge, or by providing information in the initial premises that heavily primed a subject to arrive at a given conclusion. Certain arguments were also counterbalanced so that half of the arguments were anticipated to draw a conclusion of ‘probably yes’ and the other half were expected to draw the conclusion of ‘probably no’. To summarize, all arguments were divided into four overall conditions: emotional uncertain [EU], neutral uncertain [NU], emotional certain [EC] and neutral certain [NC]. Every attempt was made to create arguments in which neither Theory of Mind nor moral reasoning would be recruited to arrive at the conclusion. For example, when the arguments involved other people, subjects were asked to reason about non-mentalizing aspects of them [i.e. physical/material traits about the individual, or the likelihood of said individual surviving a disease or an accident]. Critically, each of the four conditions contained an equal number of arguments in which people were involved [32 arguments per condition]. This was done because certain investigations have found that reasoning about others, even when not reasoning about another’s mental state, can involve the medial prefrontal cortex [Saxe and Powell, 2006], an a priori region of interest in this experiment. The remaining arguments [32 per condition] did not contain themes about people, rather, these centered on various themes such as animals, abstract historical events, or physical objects [each of which was also equal in number across conditions]. Finally, all words in the syllogistic arguments were compared across conditions using a psycholinguistic database [www.psy.uwa.edu.au/mrcdatabase/uwa_mrc.htm]. The arguments did not differ statistically across the four conditions with respect to the following: amount of letters [F = 0.824, df = 3, 77, P = 0.485], amount of words [F = 0.09, df = 3, 77, P = 0.965], frequency of the words in the English language [F = 0.008, df = 3, 77, P = 0.999] and the concreteness/abstract quality of the words used [F = 0.095, df = 3, 77, P = 0.963].

Table 1

Sample uncertain inductive arguments used as stimuli

Emotional Uncertain [EU] argumentsNeutral Uncertain [NU] arguments
Amy has a fatal form of breast cancer Molly plays a rare sport
Amy has blood seeping out of her nipple Molly is rather tall
This is always a bad sign in breast cancer Most women who play this sport are tall
Ted lost fifty pounds in one month Tony has a B negative blood type
Ted did not try to lose this weight Tony can donate blood to other B negatives
Ted is probably dying He can donate blood to B positive people
Adults can go missing An average adult has a certain heart rate
Adults missing for a month are often dead An average child has a faster rate
Kids missing for a week are often dead An average baby has an even faster rate
A mother and father died at the same time Max has a certain occupation
Their son was with them at that moment Max is constantly writing at work
Their son probably died with them Most people who have this job write a lot
Premature babies weigh very little Babies tend to sleep more than adults
They can get a massive brain bleed and die Dreams occur in a stage of sleep
Most low weight babies are prone to this Babies tend to dream more than adults
Liz had three miscarriages A woman cannot see the color green
Liz then gave birth to a stillborn baby She lacks the gene needed for this
Liz will not have a normal pregnancy The woman is unable to see other colors
The World Trade Center was destroyed Some stars shine brighter than others
A big smoke cloud was visible miles away Light from stars has to travel very far
The smoke could be seen from space The brighter stars are closer to Earth
A lethal injection contains potassium A chemical keeps produce fresh
Injecting a big dose of potassium is fatal A second chemical is structurally similar
Swallowing a big dose is also fatal This chemical also keeps produce fresh
The bible condemns homosexuality Several cave paintings were found
A rare religion is similar to Christianity Many of the paintings depicted the sun
Its holy book condemns homosexuality They were made by a sun worshipping culture
The AIDS virus is hard to kill A type of bird eats only nuts
This is because the virus mutates a lot The bird type has a specialized beak
Most deadly viruses can mutate a lot All birds with this beak eat only nuts
Monkeys can feel intense pain Some fish live on the ocean floor
The rat nervous system is much simpler There is virtually no light there
Rats feel pain less than monkeys do These fish are blind
Many unwanted cats are put in shelters Flowers are important to bumblebees
Even more dogs are put in shelters Bees can discriminate many flower colors
More dogs are killed in shelters each year Bees must have good vision in general

Table 2

Sample certain inductive arguments used as stimuli

Emotional Certain [EC] argumentsNeutral Certain [NC] arguments
‘Certain’ arguments in which a conclusion of ‘yes’ was expected
A young couple is extremely poor An athlete broke a world record
They sometimes have to eat dog food The record was broken in a certain sport
They probably do not live in a nice house The athlete is probably good at that sport
A man got stabbed many times in the face Jeremiah has a red colored beard
He also got stabbed many times in the heart Jeremiah also has red colored eyebrows
The man probably died Jeremiah might have red hair
A very thin girl with anorexia died Alexander has Russian parents
She looked like a concentration camp victim Alexander also has Russian siblings
She may have died from starvation Alexander might also be Russian
A baby was kidnapped and then abandoned A boy is in elementary school
She was left alone for an entire week The boy wears a certain shirt size
She might have died as a result He likely wears smaller shirts than his dad
A rope was used to commit suicide Airplanes are designed a certain way
The rope was tied into a noose They all have the same shape
The rope was used in a hanging This shape probably facilitates flying
A gas was synthesized An animal lives in the north pole
It was made for use in a gas chamber This animal has a very thick fur coat
The gas is probably poisonous The animal can tolerate cold weather
‘Certain’ arguments in which a conclusion of ‘no’ was expected
Jeff was murdered during a hate crime A woman was born and raised in America
Jeff was not Jewish The woman now drives a Japanese car
Thus Jews are never victims of hate crimes Therefore she must now live in Japan
Kim snapped her neck in a diving accident Spanish is a common language in America
Kim is still able to move her hands Elizabeth is an American citizen
Broken necks never cause total paralysis Thus Elizabeth must speak Spanish
Retarded people are never smart Wrinkles are a sign of aging
Smart people can contribute to society Amanda just got her first wrinkle
Retarded people never contribute to society Thus Amanda must be over ninety years old
A restaurant kitchen is filled with roaches A dog from a certain breed was observed
This roach species is not poisonous The dog was noted to be a male
Roaches can safely crawl on the food Females must not exist in this breed
Anthrax is a biological weapon A battery held its charge for one week
It usually causes death via inhalation Another battery still works after two weeks
Thus it is probably safe to ingest The second battery will work forever
Terrorism occurs in the name of Islam An average coin was flipped three times
The Muslim religion is based on a holy book Each time the coin came up heads
This book only preaches violence This means it will always come up heads

Stimuli presentation

Each syllogism was presented for a total of 12 s [12000 ms]. The first sentence was presented for 3000 ms, after which the second sentence appeared and was presented for an additional 3000 ms. The third sentence then appeared at time t = 6000 ms and remained on the screen for an additional 6000 ms, during which time subjects were instructed to respond with a button press. The first two sentences remained on the screen for the duration of the trial [Figure 1]. The 256 syllogisms were presented over the course of four functional runs [64 trials per run]. In each functional run, the same number of syllogisms [16] was presented for each of the four conditions. Trial [syllogism] presentation was done in a pseudo-random fashion using an optimization sequence program. The inter-trial interval [ITI] ranged from 0 ms to 12 000 ms.

Stimuli presentation. Each syllogism was presented for a total of 12 s [12000 ms]. The first premise was presented for 3000 ms, after which the second premise appeared and was presented for an additional 3000 ms. The third premise [the conclusion] then appeared at time t = 6000 ms and remained on the screen for an additional 6000 ms, during which time subjects were instructed to respond with a button press. The BOLD signal was modeled as a hemodynamic response function with SOAs assigned at the temporal midpoint between the presentation of the third sentence and the motor response [i.e. the button press].

Post-scan ratings

Immediately following the scanning procedure subjects completed a questionnaire in which they re-read each of the 256 syllogistic arguments. For each of these, subjects were again asked to rate the argument as ‘probably true’ or ‘probably false’. Next, for each argument subjects were asked to rate the percent certainty of their answer [on a scale of 0–100, 0% being completely unsure and 100% being completely certain]. In addition, subjects rated the valence and arousal of each syllogism read in the scanner via the use of the Self-Manikin Assessment Scale [Lang, 1980]. This is a 9-point scale in which a score of 1 corresponds to extremely negative valence and a score of 9 corresponds to extremely positive valence. In a similar fashion, with respect to arousal ratings, a score of 1 indicated very low arousal and a score of 9 indicated extremely high arousal. Lastly, behavioral measures also included reaction times [computed as mean reaction times across the four functional runs].

fMRI acquisition

MRI data were acquired using a 3.0-T whole-body scanner [Allegra; Siemens Medical Solutions], equipped for echo planar imaging [Siemens Medical Systems, Iselin, NJ, USA] with a 12 channel 3-axis gradient head coil. Head movements were restricted using foam cushions. Images were projected using a rear projection system. Following automated scout and shimming procedures, two high-resolution 3D MPRAGE sequences [TR = 2.53 ms, TE = 3.45 ms, flip angle = 7°, voxel size = 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.3 mm] were collected for positioning of subsequent scans. fMRI images [i.e. blood oxygenation level dependent signal or BOLD] were acquired using T2*-weighted sequences [TR = 3000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, voxel size = 3.1 × 3.1 × 5.0 mm, slice thickness = 5.0 mm, FoV = 200 mm, number of slices = 27]. The paradigm included four functional runs, each lasting 960 s [with each containing 320 image volumes].

fMRI Data analysis

Functional Data were processed using SPM5 software [Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London, UK; www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm]. fMRI images were motion corrected, spatially normalized to the standardized space established by the Montreal Neurologic Institute [MNI; www.bic.mni.mcgill.ca], re-sampled to 2 mm3 voxels, and smoothed with a three-dimensional Gaussian kernel of 6 mm width [FWHM]. All collected data had minimal head motion [ neutral trials was partly due to the fact these arguments were generally easier to resolve.

fMRI data

Main effect of reasoning

The main effect of reasoning vs baseline [[EU + EC + NU + NC] − Fixation] resulted in activation of posterior medial cortex, inferior frontal cortex, lingual gyrus and subcortical structures such as the caudate nucleus [Table 5]. These areas are concordant to those found as a main effect of reasoning by Goel and Dolan [2003] in the study upon which the current study was based, as well as in other investigations by these authors [Goel and Dolan, 2000].

Table 5

Brain regions activated in conjunction with all four conditions vs fixation

RegionBrodmann areaCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Posterior dorsomedial PFC 8 597 2 26 46 7.11
Right inferior frontal gyrus 47 221 34 24 −8 5.82
Right inferior frontal gyrus 45 52 48 14 16 5.76
Right caudate nucleus N/A 34 16 4 12 5.95
Left inferior frontal gyrus 47 25 −52 22 −2 4.72
Left lingual gyrus 18 24 −4 −80 −4 6.44
Right anterior thalamus N/A 23 10 −6 4 6.04
Left insular cortex 13 21 −48 −18 22 5.71

Main effect of emotion

Based on a whole brain analysis, the main effect of emotion [EU + EC] > [NU + NC] revealed robust activation in established regions of the DN, including the PCC [BA 31] [−12, −50, 34; T = 11.20, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]. In addition, there appeared to be more specific activation of the dorsomedial subsystem of the network, namely: the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex [BA 9] [−4, 54, 28] [T = 12.60, PFWE-corrected < 0.001], the left TPJ [BA 40] [−54, −52, 32; T = 9.24, PFWE-corrected < 0.001], and the right lateral temporal cortex [[BA 21] [52,−18,−14; T = 7.50, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]] [Tables 6 and 7 and Figure 2]. Notably, there was no activation of ‘ventral’ MPFC, even at much lower cluster thresholds. Given the emotional nature of the stimuli employed, a separate ROI analysis revealed that bilateral amygdala [[22, −8, −12, z = 4.17, PFWE-corrected = 0.002] [−26, 0, −16, T = 4.53 PFWE-corrected = 0.003]] were also activated by the main effect of emotion. However, using a ROI analysis with the same threshold level [kE ≥ 10 exceeding a statistical threshold of P < 0.05 [FWE corrected]] there was no activation in other medial temporal structures [e.g. the PHC and hippocampus]. The contrast of neutral trials minus emotional trials [NU + NC] > [EU + EC] resulted in far less activation at the aforementioned threshold, with activation of only the Right superior parietal lobule [BA 7] [30, −68, 50] [T = 5.96, PFWE-corrected < 0.001].

Main effect of emotion. SPMs of voxel-wise T-scores at a minimum significance of P < 0.05 FWE corrected, and with a minimum cluster size of 75 contiguous voxels. SPM maps were 3D rendered onto a standardized template. Main effect of emotional salience on reasoning [EU + EC] > [NU + NC] shows activation in the dMPFC subsystem of the DN including: dMPFC [BA 9], Right TPJ [BA 39] and lateral temporal cortex LTC [BA 21]. There was no activation in the MTL subsystem as a result of this contrast. Figures generated with MRIcron [//www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricron/index.html].

Table 6

Brain regions showing activation as a result of the main effect of emotion

RegionBrodmann areaNetwork affiliationCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Dorsomedial PFC 9 dMPFC subsystem 1647 −4 54 28 12.60
Precuneus 31 DN core hub 738 −12 −50 34 11.20
Left TPJ [Supramarginal gyrus] 40 dMPFC subsystem 475 −54 −52 32 9.24
Left middle temporal cortex 21 dMPFC subsystem 358 −52 −18 −14 6.23
Left inferior frontal gyrus 47 None 248 −44 26 −10 7.81
Right TPJ [Sup temp gyrus] 39 dMPFC subsystem 126 50 −58 24 7.06

Table 7

Brain regions showing activation as a result of the main effect of emotion

RegionCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Right and left amygdala 10 22 −8 12 4.53
37 −26 0 −16 4.38

Main effects of uncertainty and certainty on reasoning

The main effect of uncertainty during reasoning [i.e. Uncertainty [EU + NU] > Certainty [EC + NC]], irrespective of emotional valence, was activation of established executive and attentional control areas. For example, this contrast revealed activation in a rather posterior portion of the medial prefrontal cortex: [BA 8] [−4, 24, 48; T = 10.12, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]. There was also marked activation in left dorsolateral PFC [lateral portions of BA 9 and BA 10] [[−50, 18, 28; T = 8.23, PFWE-corrected < 0.001] and [−42, 48, 0; T = 8.33, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]] [Table 8 and Figure 3]. Of note, a similar trend of activating executive control centers was observed when uncertainty level was increased ‘within’ emotional arguments [e.g. EU > EC].

Main effects of uncertainty [A] and certainty [B]. SPMs of voxel-wise T-scores at a minimum significance of P < 0.05 FWE corrected and with a minimum cluster size of 75 contiguous voxels are rendered onto standardized coronal, sagittal and axial sections [clockwise from top left]. [A] Main effect of uncertainty revealed activation in posterior medial prefrontal cortex [BA 8] and Left DLPFC [lateral BA 9]. [B] Main effect of certainty activated DN regions including the precuneus and aMPFC. It also activated some areas specific to the MTL subsystem of the DN, including the RsP and vMPFC, as well as hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus [based upon an ROI analysis [not shown]]. Figures generated with MRIcron [//www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricron/index.html].

Table 8

Brain regions showing activation as a result of the main effect of uncertainty

RegionBrodmann areaCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Posterior dorsomedial PFC 8 438 −4 24 48 10.12
Left DLPFC 9 897 −50 18 28 8.23
Left inferior frontal gyrus 10 235 −42 48 0 8.33

The main effect of certainty, [i.e. Certainty [EC + NC] > Uncertainty [EU + NU]], resulted in significant activation of key hubs of the DN, specifically: right supramarginal gyrus [BA 40] [58, −46, 32; T = 11.58, PFWE-corrected < 0.001], precuneus [BA 31] [−14, −64, 28; T = 10.66, PFWE-corrected < 0.001], retrosplenial cortex [BA 23] [4, −42, 22; T = 9.10, PFWE-corrected < 0.001], anterior medial prefrontal cortex [aMPFC] [BA 10] [12, 48,−2; T = 7.10, PFWE-corrected < 0.001] and vMPFC [BA 24] [−6, 8, 38; T = 6.75, PFWE-corrected = 0.001] [Tables 9 and 10 and Figure 3]. Because of the a priori hypothesis that reasoning with certainty might recruit medial temporal structures, we performed a ROI analysis of HF and PHC. This revealed activation in the Right hippocampus [28, −28, −10; T = 6.05, PFWE-corrected < 0.001, kE = 55] and bilateral parahippocampal gyri [BA 34] [[22, 2, 16; T = 6.26, PFWE-corrected < 0.001, kE = 47] and [−24, 2,−14; T = 3.99, PFWE-corrected < 0.001, kE = 11]]. We also noted activation of areas of the DN when certainty was increased within emotional arguments [e.g. EC > EU]. This contrast revealed robust activation in both PCC [BA 31 [6, −66, 28; T = 9.57, kE = 1017, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]] and in vMPFC [BA 10 [0, 58, 10; T = 7.63, kE = 752, PFWE-corrected < 0.001]].

Table 9

Brain regions showing activation as a result of the main effect of certainty

RegionBrodmann areaNetwork affiliationCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Rsp 23 MTL Subsystem 2729 4 −42 22 9.10
Precuneus 31 DN core hub 1726 −14 −64 28 10.66
Right TPJ [Supramarginal gyrus] 40 dMPFC Subsystem 989 58 −46 32 11.58
aMPFC 32 DN core hub 408 −12 48 −2 7.10
vMPFC 24 MTL Subsystem 93 −6 8 38 6.75

Table 10

Brain regions showing activation as a result of the main effect of certainty

RegionBrodmann areaNetwork affiliationCluster sizeMNI-coordinatesaMax voxel T-scorebKExyz
Right hippocampus NA MTL Subsystem 55 28 −28 −10 6.05
Right PHC 34 MTL Subsystem 47 22 2 16 6.26
Left PHC 34 11 −24 2 −14 3.99

DISCUSSION

Emotional reasoning relative to neutral reasoning drives activation in the DN

The DN is characterized by several cortical regions, including the medial wall of the prefrontal cortex, the precuneus, the posterior cingulate [PCC] and retrosplenial cortex [RsP], the lateral temporal cortex [LTC] and the bilateral inferior parietal cortices; all of which were initially highlighted on the basis of common deactivations occurring during the execution of active cognitive tasks and reciprocal activations during rest [Gusnard and Raichle, 2001; Raichle et al., 2001; Raichle, 2006; Buckner et al., 2008]. The current investigation substantiates a role for this network in certain forms of content-specific reasoning. Specifically, the authors observed activation in areas of the DN both when participants reasoned about emotional [relative to neutral] themes, and when they reasoned in the context of certainty [relative to uncertainty]. Of note, the authors do not posit that the DN has a privileged role in resolving logical arguments-only that it is mobilized for certain forms of logical arguments-specifically, those that invoke the hypothesized cognitive functions of the network. To illustrate this, previous investigators have demonstrated activation in the DN for tasks of moral reasoning and Theory of Mind reasoning [ToM] relative to control tasks [Amodio and Firth, 2006; Saxe et al., 2004; Greene et al., 2001; Greene et al., 2004].

In order to interpret the observed BOLD activations in the DN during the emotional conditions it is necessary to elaborate upon the potential effects of emotional content upon the reasoning process. For example, the case could be made that the DN activation we report was reflective of a ‘cognitive’ reappraisal of an induced affective state. In other words, perhaps the emotional nature of the syllogisms served as a source of distraction or load, and, as a result, the observed activation in the DN reflected some form of conscious emotional regulation to modulate or suppress this. We argue that this was not the case. Unlike other studies that have been designed to induce ‘explicit’ emotional regulation [Ochsner and Gross, 2005; Hariri et al., 2003], our study involved ‘implicit’ emotional regulation. That is, participants were not asked to regulate their emotional reaction to the arguments, but were rather instructed to integrate all of the presented information [whether emotional or neutral] into deriving their conclusion. The distinction between explicit and implicit affect regulation was eloquently described in a study of motivated political reasoning by Westen and colleagues [2006]. These authors explained that in reasoning underwritten by implicit affect regulation, there is a tendency for the brain to derive conclusions which achieve a maximally positive [and minimally negative] affective state, while still attempting to satisfy ‘cognitive’ constraints of the argument. Similar to existing integrative models of cognition and emotion [Pessoa, 2008; Gray et al., 2002], we posit that the DN was not activated simply as a byproduct of the stimuli being emotional, or as a way of down-regulating the induced affective state, but instead was recruited to implicitly ‘integrate’ the emotional data contained in the premises into the process of deriving a logical induction.

There is extant evidence to support a role for the DN in emotional reasoning. First, the DN is anatomically composed of, or highly interconnected to, regions which are critical for processing emotional and motivational data [Gusnard et al., 2001], and Raichle [Raichle and Gusnard, 2005] has opined that, ‘it is highly likely that [intrinsic functional activity in the brain] will be increasingly recognized as playing an important role in the ultimate shaping and expression of basic appetites and drives’. Second, a role for the network in emotional processing in general has been experimentally substantiated. For instance, one investigation found that DN activity is modulated on the basis of whether musical sounds are neutral or unpleasant [Pallesen et al., 2008], and another study found alterations in resting state functional connectivity with sad mood induction [Harrison et al., 2008]. Also, depressed patients and those suffering from chronic pain are less capable of suppressing their DN [Sheline et al., 2009, Grimm et al., 2009 and Baliki et al., 2008], and such patients appear to have altered functional connectivity between DN regions [Greicius et al., 2007, Anand et al., 2005].

Emotional reasoning specifically activates the dorsomedial subsystem of the DN

Our results also demonstrate that the dorsomedial subsystem of the DN was preferentially recruited by emotional reasoning, as the Main Effect of Emotion activated dMPFC, TPJ and LTC, while failing to activate any of the components specific to the MTL subsystem. A myriad of investigations have linked the dMPFC to self-referential processing [Lane et al., 1997; Johnson et al., 2002; Gusnard et al., 2001; Vanderwal et al., 2008], and to the metacognitive appraisal of one’s emotional state [Seeley and Sturm, 2007; Ochsner and Gross, 2005]. In their meta-analysis, Kober et al. [2008] found that dMPFC was most likely among medial prefrontal regions to co-activate with the hypothalamus and the periaqueductal gray, which themselves serve critical roles in the physiological mediation of emotional states. Moreover, Andrews-Hanna et al. have provided evidence that tasks which accentuate ‘self-other’ distinctions specifically activate the entire dorsomedial subsystem [not just the dMPFC]. In the current paradigm, we posit that the dMPFC subsystem was activated during emotional trials in order to mobilize introspective/self-relevant data to arrive at a conclusion. By this argument, the emotional syllogisms employed were inherently more ‘personal’ or ‘self-relevant’ than the neutral ones, as they often involved people being ill, injured or dying [Tables 1 and 2]. This is not to say, however, that the activations observed in the dMPFC subsystem were solely based upon a personal/impersonal distinction, as the emotional stimuli used did not have direct relevance to the participants on an individual level [as is the case with other studies which probe self/non-self distinctions].

Interestingly, at the statistical thresholds employed, vMPFC was not highlighted by the main effect of emotion. This is in contrast to the findings of vMPFC activation during emotionally valenced deductive reasoning in the Goel and Dolan [2003] experiment. However, when using a different intensity threshold [FDR correction], we did detect activation in vMPFC as a function of the Main Effect of Emotion [BA 11 [0, 46, −16; T = 4.57, kE = 45, PFDR-corrected = 0.003]]. Our results also differ from those of Goel and Dolan in that these authors did not observe activations in other DN areas, such as TPJ, LTC, PCC and dMPFC. We argue that this discrepancy can be explained by two considerations. First, as noted in the Introduction, there is evidence to link dMPFC to inductive, and not deductive reasoning. Second, it is quite possible that our emotional stimuli were more empathetic than those employed by Goel and Dolan, as they often involved graphic depictions of specific individuals or animals suffering. Meta-analyses have linked empathic processing to nodes of the dMPFC subsystem, such as dMPFC itself and TPJ [Lamm et al., 2011; Amodio and Firth, 2006]. Also, Decety and Chaminade [2003] found that sympathy is associated with activation in dMPFC. Finally, our study is not alone in finding activation in DN areas during emotional reasoning. The above referenced study of motivated reasoning by Westen et al. [2006] found robust activation in PCC and in dMPFC, in addition to vMPFC.

Certainty and uncertainty differentially engage and disengage the DN, respectively

Increases in uncertainty during reasoning were associated with increased activity in a posterior region of the MPFC [BA 8], as well as with activation of left DLPFC [a lateral portion of BA 9] [Figure 3]. Notably, this was true even within emotional trials [e.g. in the contrast of EU-EC]. These regions are quite similar to those found in a study on the neural basis of uncertainty during decision-making by Volz et al. [2004]. Specifically, those authors demonstrated activation in posterior medial frontal cortex [BA 8] during uncertainty and additional activation in DLPFC when said uncertainty was internally attributed. Moreover, in a meta-analysis, Ridderinkhof and colleagues [2004] implicated the posterior medial frontal cortex [BA 8] in the reconciliation of uncertainty and the detection of response conflict. The DLPFC has often been associated with executive control tasks during which information must be continually updated and/or manipulated [e.g. via the utilization of working memory or set shifting] [Mansouri et al., 2009; Stuss and Alexander, 2000]. Therefore, activation of posterior MPFC and DLPFC during the resolution of uncertainty seems concordant with established theories about these regions.

Certainty [when contrasted with uncertainty] during reasoning also yielded activations in the DN. Specifically, we observed activations in aMPFC and in PCC. One possible interpretation of this trend is that certain trials were less difficult than uncertain trials. Several prior observations have shown that deactivations in the DN become more pronounced as the level of task difficulty is increased [Pallesen et al., 2009; Greicius and Menon, 2004], and these deactivations have been shown to be proportional to the degree of task difficulty in a parametric fashion [McKiernan et al., 2003]. This phenomenon is often explained via a resource allocation model as proposed by McKiernan et al.

However, we put forth an alternative explanation: that areas of the DN were recruited during certain trials because of the nature of this type of reasoning. Certain trials were designed to elicit the use of prior knowledge and/or well established semantic/mnemonic associations. Interestingly, the Main Effect of Certainty also activated areas selective to the MTL subsystem of the DN, including HF, PHC, RsP and vMPFC. This finding resonates with prior studies. As mentioned in the introduction, Bar and colleagues [2007] have shown that reasoning which relies upon established semantic associations specifically activates the medial temporal portions of the network. Further, there is extant evidence linking medial temporal structures [e.g. HF and PHC] to reasoning based which is based on episodic memory [Girelli et al., 2004; Suzuki et al., 2009].

To disentangle the contributions of task difficulty and certainty to DN [and MTL subsystem] activation, we parametrically weighted the four conditions of interest [EU, NU, EC and NC] by both the subject’s trial-by-trial certainty ratings, and by their trial-by-trial reaction times [with the assumption that reaction time is a proxy of task difficulty]. Parametric weighting by certainty scores revealed activation in the MTL subsystem of the DN as a result of the Main Effect of Certainty in: left PHC [−32, −30, −14; kE = 23, T = 4.12, uncorrected P < 0.001], right HF [30, −6, −28; kE = 3, T = 3.51, uncorrected P < 0.001] and right pIPL [44, −30, −14; kE = 15, T = 4.12, uncorrected P < 0.005] on a whole brain analysis. In contrast, parametric weighting by reaction time did not reveal activation in the MTL subsystem or in the DN, but rather resulted in activation in the left caudate nucleus [−8, 6, 18; kE = 30, T = 4.63, uncorrected P < 0.001] and the right premotor area [12, 26, 50; kE = 32, T = 3.23, uncorrected P < 0.005]. These findings suggest that DN/MTL subsystem activation occurring during certainty reasoning was the result of the certainty of the subject’s conclusions, above and beyond the effect of difficulty level. Nonetheless, these two factors are inherently interrelated, and as such, a limitation of our study is that our design did not allow for them to be directly compared.

CONCLUSION

The DN is characterized by of a nexus of brain regions with remarkably strong functional inter-connectedness consuming a large amount of the brain’s energy budget [Raichle, 2006]. Investigations have linked reasoning in certain contexts to activation in the DN, and the DN has often been linked to probability assessment. Our findings suggest that the DN is mobilized during probability assessments [as reflected by inductive conclusions] when emotional salience is involved, and when there is certainty about the conclusion being drawn. We also show that these two types of reasoning specifically mobilize different subsystems of the DN [Figure 4], with the dMPFC subsystem being activated during emotional reasoning and the MTL subsystem being activated during certainty reasoning. Activation of the dMPFC subsystem of the DN during emotional reasoning suggests that participants arrived at emotional conclusions based in part upon self-relevant or introspective cues. One caveat to these findings is that the emotional stimuli used were all negative [and not positive] in content. As such, another potential limitation of this study is that the observed activations in the dMPFC subsystem were the result of the effects of negative valence upon reasoning and not emotionality in general. Reasoning under certainty, in contrast, activated components of the MTL subsystem, raising the possibility that this subsystem might assist in making judgments rooted in well learned information or in episodic memory. We argue that future investigations of the neural basis of human reasoning should focus on network models. We also posit that the DN may play an important role in the integration of cognitive and implicit affective processing.

Anatomical overlap of the main effects of emotion and certainty upon reasoning. Both the Main Effects of certainty [shown in green/blue] and emotion [shown in red/yellow] activated components of the DN. However, the main effect of emotion activated a more dorsal portion of the medial prefrontal cortex [in addition to other components of the dorsomedial subsystem], whereas the main effect of certainty activated a more ventral portion. SPMs of voxel-wise T-scores at a minimum significance of P < 0.05 FWE corrected and with a minimum cluster size of 75 contiguous voxels are rendered onto standardized coronal, sagittal and axial sections [clockwise from top left]. Figures generated with MRIcron [//www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricron/index.html].

Conflict of Interest

None declared.

Acknowledgments

M.C.E. formulated the hypothesis, assisted with the study design, collected the data, performed the analyses and wrote the article. L.E.C. assisted with data collection and data analyses. J.C.B. assisted with data collection and analyses for the article revisions. T.D. and D.D.D. assisted with hypothesis formulation and study design, and oversaw all aspects of data collection and analysis. All authors reviewed and commented on the article. This work was supported by a pilot grant from the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging of the Massachusetts General Hospital via support from the ‘Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies’ [P41RR14075]. In addition, M.C.E. was supported by the Mind, Brain and Behavior Interfaculty Initiative at Harvard University.

REFERENCES

  • Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2006;7[4]:268–77. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Anand A, Li Y, Wang Y, et al. Activity and connectivity of brain mood regulating circuit in depression: a functional magnetic resonance study. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57[10]:1079–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Andrews-Hanna JR, Riedler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron. 2010;65:550–62. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baliki MN, Geha PY, Apkarian AV, Chialvo DR. Beyond feeling: chronic pain hurts the brain, disrupting the default-mode network dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28[6]:1398–403. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bar M. Predictions: a universal principle in the operation of the human brain. Introduction. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological sciences. 2009;364[1521]:1181–2. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bar M, Aminoff E, Mason M, Fenske M. The units of thought. Hippocampus. 2007;17[6]:420–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997;275[5304]:1293–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition: Manual. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • Blanchette I. The effect of emotion on interpretation and logic in a conditional reasoning task. Memory and Cognition. 2006;34[5]:1112–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Blanchette I, Richards A. Reasoning about emotional and neutral materials. Is logic affected by emotion? Psychological Sciences. 2004;15[11]:745–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2008;1124:1–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Corcoran R. The allusive cognitive deficit in paranoia: the case for mental time travel or cognitive self-projection. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40[8]:1233–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Damasio AR. The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological sciences. 1996;351[1346]:1413–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Decety J, Chaminade T. Neural correlates of feeling sympathy. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41[2]:127–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Patient Edition [SCID-I/D, Version 2.0] New York: Biometrics Research Department; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • Fellows LK. Advances in understanding ventromedial prefrontal function: the accountant joins the executive. Neurology. 2007;68[13]:991–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Girelli L, Semenza C, Delazer M. Inductive reasoning and implicit memory: evidence from intact and impaired memory systems. Neuropsychologia. 2004;42[7]:926–38. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V. Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2007a;11[10]:435–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V, Dolan RJ. Anatomical segregation of component processes in an inductive inference task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2000;12[1]:110–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V, Dolan RJ. Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning. Neuroimage. 2003;20[4]:2314–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V, Dolan RJ. Differential involvement of left prefrontal cortex in inductive and deductive reasoning. Cognition. 2004;93[3]:B109–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V, Gold B, Kapur S, Houle S. The seats of reason? An imaging study of deductive and inductive reasoning. Neuroreport. 1997;8[5]:1305–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Goel V, Tierney M, Sheesley L, Bartolo A, Vartanian O, Grafman J. Hemispheric specialization in human prefrontal cortex for resolving certain and uncertain inferences. Cereb Cortex. 2007b;17:2245–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gray JR, Braver TS, Raichle ME. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA. 2002;99[6]:4115–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Greene JD, Nystrom LE, Engell AD, Darley JM, Cohen JD. The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. Neuron. 2004;44[2]:389–400. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science. 2001;293[5537]:2105–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Greicius MD, Flores BH, Menon V, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;62[5]:429–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Greicius MD, Menon V. Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2004;16[9]:1484–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grimm S, Boesiger P, Beck J, et al. Altered negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network during emotion processing in depressed subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34[4]:932–843. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gusnard DA, Akbudak E, Shulman GL, Raichle ME. Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: relation to a default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2001;98[7]:4259–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nature Review Neuroscience. 2001;2[10]:685–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry. 1960;23:56–62. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hansel A, von Kanel R. The ventro-medial prefrontal cortex: a major link between the autonomic nervous system, regulation of emotion, and stress reactivity? Biopsychosocial Medicine. 2008;2:21. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Fera F, Weinberger DR. Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli. Biological Psychiatry. 2003;53[6]:494–501. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harrison BJ, Pujol J, Ortiz H, Fornito A, Pantelis C, Yucel M. Modulation of brain resting-state networks by sad mood induction. PLoS One. 2008;3[3]:e1794. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson SC, Baxter LC, Wilder LS, Pipe JG, Heiserman JE, Prigatano GP. Neural correlates of self-reflection. Brain. 2002;125:1808–14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kober H, Barrett LF, Joseph J, Bliss-Moreau E, Lindquist K, Wager TD. Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2008;42[2]:998–1031. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Knutson B, Taylor J, Kaufman M, Peterson R, Glover G. Distributed neural representation of expected value. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25[19]:4806–12. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage. 2011;54[3]:2492–502. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lane RD, Fink GR, Chau PM, Dolan RJ. Neural activation during selective attention to subjective emotional responses. Neuroreport. 1997;8:3969–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lang PJ. Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral assessment: computer applications. In: Sidowski JB, Johnson JH, Williams TA, editors. Technology in Mental Health Care Delivery Systems. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1980. pp. 119–37. [Google Scholar]
  • Mansouri FA, Tanaka K, Buckley MJ. Conflict-induced behavioural adjustment: a clue to the executive functions of the prefrontal cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10[2]:141–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McKiernan KA, Kaufman JN, Kucera-Thompson J, Binder JR. A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15[3]:394–408. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McClure SM, Laibson DI, Lowenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Oaksford M, Chater N. The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2001;5[8]:349–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2005;9[5]:242–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pallesen KJ, Brattico E, Bailey CJ, Korvenoja A, Gjedde A. Cognitive and emotional modulation of brain default operation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21[6]:1065–80. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pessoa L. On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Review Neuroscience. 2008;9[2]:148–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Raichle ME. Neuroscience. The brain's dark energy. Science. 2006;314[5803]:1249–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Raichle ME, Gusnard DA. Intrinsic brain activity sets the stage for expression of motivated behavior. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2005;493[1]:167–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 16. 2001;98[2]:676–82. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306[5695]:443–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rodriguez-Moreno D, Hirsch J. The dynamics of deductive reasoning: an fMRI investigation. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47[4]:949–61. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Saxe R, Carey S, Kanwisher N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Reviews Psychology. 2004;55:87–124. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Saxe R, Powell LJ. It's the thought that counts: specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science. 2006;17[8]:692–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schneider F, Bermpohl F, Heinzel A, et al. The resting brain and our self: self-relatedness modulates resting state neural activity in cortical midline structures. Neuroscience. 2008;157[1]:120–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Seeley WM, Sturm VE. Self-representation and the frontal lobes. In: Miller BL, Cummings JL, editors. The Human Frontal Lobes. 2nd edn. New York: The Guilford Press; 2007. pp. 317–34. [Google Scholar]
  • Sheline YI, Barch DM, Price JL, et al. The default mode network and self-referential processes in depression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA. 2009;106[6]:1942–7. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Stuss DT, Alexander MP. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Pschological Research. 2000;63[3–4]:289–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Suzuki C, Tsukiura T, Mochizuki-Kawai H, Shigemune Y, Iijima T. Prefrontal and medial temporal contributions to episodic memory-based reasoning. Neuroscience Research. 2009;63:177–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vanderwal T, Hunyadi E, Grupe DW, Connors CM, Schultz RT. Self, mother and abstract other: an fMRI study of reflective social processing. Neuroimage. 2008;41:1437–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Volz KG, Schubotz RI, von Cramon DY. Why am I unsure? Internal and external attributions of uncertainty dissociated by fMRI. Neuroimage. 2004;21[3]:848–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Westen D, Blagov PS, Harenski K, Kilts C, Hamann S. Neural bases of motivated reasoning: an FMRI study of emotional constraints on partisan political judgment in the 2004 U.S. Presidential election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18[11]:1947–58. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

Which of the following words can be used in a logical appeal?

Which of the following words can be used in a logical appeal? Analysis : is the word that can be used in a logical appeal. These appeals are based on the reader's notions of reason. Freedom is a word that can be used in an emotional appeal.

Which type of reasoning does a logical appeal use when working from specific evidence to a general conclusion?

Inductive reasoning takes a specific representative case or facts and then draws generalizations or conclusions from them. Inductive reasoning must be based on a sufficient amount of reliable evidence.

What does it mean to use emotional appeals ethically?

Emotional appeals often manipulate people's emotions in order to persuade, and ethical appeals rely on qualities that might not pass the truth test. Logical appeals, which present facts and evidence, focus on the truth.

Is a logical fallacy in which one tries to support one's claim by restating it in different words?

Logical appeal uses one of three types of reasoning—analogy, induction, and deduction. Analogy refers to a logical fallacy in which you try to support your claim by reinstating it in different words.

Chủ Đề